Press "Enter" to skip to content

Science Reveals: How Does Your Brain Be Creative?

Two semantic memory search processes that are important in creativity have been discovered by researchers.

How can we use knowledge exploration to spark creativity?

We must use all of our prior knowledge while trying to come up with a creative idea. But how does this take place in our thoughts and brains? Two semantic memory search mechanisms that are involved in creativity have been uncovered by Emmanuelle Volle’s group (Inserm) at the Frontlab of the Paris Brain Institute in association with the Universities of Graz (Austria), Warwick (UK), and the Israel Institute of Technology.

Creativity is not something that just happens. But it’s still a mystery how creative thoughts develop in our minds. According to current theories, it is partly dependent on how our information is organized in semantic memory and how we search for concepts there.

“What actually happens when we look for a new idea? Until now, we didn’t have a clear idea about the processes that allow us to navigate our semantic memory and be creative,” explains Marcela Ovando-Tellez, a postdoctoral fellow at Frontlab and the first author of the study.

Semantic memory and creativity

Semantic memory may be seen as a network of associations between things and ideas that are more or less connected to one another. For instance, the word “apple” will be closely related to the category of “fruit,” as well as to the ideas of “sweet,” “vegetable,” and even further-off phrases like “fairytale” (if you have read Snow White). We are able to make sense of the world because of all these concepts that are stored in our semantic memory.

The network’s structure and how we move across it are directly related to executive control procedures, and these two factors are crucial to creativity. It is simpler to come up with creative thoughts if the semantic linkages are set up such that connections between far-off items can be made with ease.

The components of the semantic memory search process: clustering and switching

In order to understand how we navigate along this network of semantic associations to unearth creative thoughts, Emmanuelle Volle’s group (Inserm) and their collaborators constructed a free semantic association task which consists of giving a cue word to a participant and asking them for all the associates that come to mind in relation to the proposed word. “The specificity here was that the cue words were polysemous, i.e., they had several possible meanings,” explains Emmanuelle Volle (Inserm), the study’s last author. “This ambiguity results in the activation of several meanings of the cue words, which allowed us to classify the responses according to the related meaning, and to distinguish two interacting components of the memory search process: clustering and switching.”

What are clustering and switching? Taking the example of a word generation task involving the category “Animals”, clustering would consist of listing successively a number of names of a subcategory of animals such as birds, while switching would involve moving from one subcategory to another, from birds to amphibians or mammals.

The task developed by the group of scientists contained, for example, the French word “rayon”, which can have several meanings: the rays of the sun, the supermarket shelves, or the bicycle spokes. Thus, if a participant proposes words associated with “ray” in relation to the weather in a row, he or she adopts a clustering type of memory search, whereas if he or she alternates between words associated with the weather and the supermarket, his or her memory search now is of a switching type.

The researchers combined this association task with a whole series of other tests measuring creativity, the judgment of semantic associations, and executive control (i.e., inhibition, working memory, etc.). Thanks to these data, they were able to reconstruct the structure of the semantic network of each participant and relate the two components of memory search to creativity, semantic memory organization, and executive control abilities. Finally, functional imaging MRI acquisitions have enabled us to explore the underlying neural correlates.

Creativity, memory search, and cognitive control

The first result obtained by the team is that clustering and switching are indeed related to creativity, but differently. Clustering is linked to divergent thinking, i.e., the free generation of ideas, while switching is related to the ability to combine distant associations between concepts. In addition, the switching component was also related to the organization of the concepts in memory and executive control abilities.

The researchers then were able to predict both clustering and switching from the participant’s brain functional connectivity and show that the two components have different brain correlates. Clustering was predicted by connectivity patterns between brain networks related to attention and executive control, suggesting that persisting on a semantic category – all the names of mammals that come to mind, for example – involves attentional processes and may be involved in creative idea generation. Switching, on the other hand, was predicted by connectivity patterns involving mainly the default network and the control network. This pattern of connectivity may support executive control processes interacting with semantic memory to explore and combine distant elements of memory.

Taken together, these results show how the alternations between exploratory search and focused attention support creativity, and provide new insights into the neurocognitive correlates of memory search related to creative cognition.

Reference: “An investigation of the cognitive and neural correlates of semantic memory search related to creative ability” by Marcela Ovando-Tellez, Mathias Benedek, Yoed N. Kenett, Thomas Hills, Sarah Bouanane, Matthieu Bernard, Joan Belo, Theophile Bieth, and Emmanuelle Volle, 16 June 2022, Communications Biology.
DOI: 10.1038/s42003-022-03547-x

Source: SciTechDaily